Are international arbitrators biased in favor of the nominating party? The answer is: probably yes! This is in part the result of what some scholars refer to as selection and affiliation effects.

Selection effect: Because the parties nominate the arbitrators, they can choose party-appointed arbitrators with the maximum predisposition towards their case. The litigant can draw from the pool of arbitrators who have shown reliability and the appropriate attitudes towards the party’s case.

Affiliation effect: Because the parties nominate the arbitrators, party-appointed arbitrators may have some predisposition to favor the nominating party. This predisposition may operate as an implicit bias; as a result, arbitrators would have a tendency to side with the nominating party even if he or she attempts to maintain neutrality and independence.

This distinction between selection and affiliation effect is very important, although the two effects are often confounded. Only the first can be directly addressed with blinding appointments—preventing nominees from knowing which party appointed them. How strong then is the affiliation effect among arbitration professionals?

In a project with Anton Strezhnev, we designed an experiment to measure affiliation effects in international arbitrators by manipulating the source of the appointment to the tribunal. We randomly assigned actual international arbitrators participating in our experimental survey to one of two conditions: a party-appointment or a blind appointment. After presenting the survey respondents with a brief vignette describing a hypothetical arbitration scenario, respondents were asked how they thought the parties’ arbitration expenses should be apportioned. By randomly manipulating the appointment source, any observed variation in response can be attributed to the affiliation of the arbitrator itself as opposed to the selection by the litigant.

Based on the results of 266 participants, we estimate that assignment to being appointed by one of the parties to the dispute made a respondent roughly 20 percentage points more likely to shift costs to the non-appointing party, suggesting a strong affiliation effect when arbitrators exercise discretion. Our results suggest that blinding could be an effective measure to mitigate affiliation effects resulting from the current practice of unilateral party appointments. To be sure, the limitations of our experiment are clear; it is unable to assess how exactly implicit biases of individual arbitrators affect outcomes of the collective body. Nevertheless, if the affiliation effect is as strong as we believe based on the experiment, a blinding mechanism could improve the practice of arbitration significantly.


________________________

To make sure you do not miss out on regular updates from the Kluwer Arbitration Blog, please subscribe here. To submit a proposal for a blog post, please consult our Editorial Guidelines.


Kluwer Arbitration
This page as PDF

One comment

  1. This is a very interesting approach. In my opinion eliminating any bias of the arbitral tribunal which potentially undermines its legitimacy strenghtens arbitration as a dispute resolution mechanism. I was wondering if blinding the appointment source can entirely deplete the affiliation effect. As mentioned in your blog, the parties choose an arbitrator with a very clear predisposition favourable to its case. I assume that once the facts and legal questions are being discussed, it becomes clear to each party-appointed arbitrator, which party appointed him or her.

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.